79 datasets found
  1. U.S. online dating service users lying on their profiles 2024

    • statista.com
    Updated Jul 22, 2024
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Statista (2024). U.S. online dating service users lying on their profiles 2024 [Dataset]. https://www.statista.com/statistics/1481187/us-online-dating-users-lying-on-profiles/
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Jul 22, 2024
    Dataset authored and provided by
    Statistahttp://statista.com/
    Time period covered
    Mar 27, 2024 - Apr 1, 2024
    Area covered
    United States
    Description

    According to a survey conducted in April 2024 in the United States, one in five online dating service users had lied about their age on their dating profile, while 14 percent had lied about their income. A further 14 percent had lied about their hobbies and interests, and 12 percent had lied about their height.

  2. Frequency of lying and cheating in the United States 2016

    • statista.com
    Updated Apr 4, 2016
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Statista (2016). Frequency of lying and cheating in the United States 2016 [Dataset]. https://www.statista.com/statistics/539802/us-frequency-of-lying/
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Apr 4, 2016
    Dataset authored and provided by
    Statistahttp://statista.com/
    Time period covered
    Feb 9, 2016 - Feb 10, 2016
    Area covered
    United States
    Description

    This statistic shows the results of a survey among adult Americans in 2016 on how often they feel the need to lie or cheat. During the survey, 13 percent of respondents stated they occasionally have to lie or to cheat.

  3. o

    Replication data for: Lying Aversion and the Size of the Lie

    • openicpsr.org
    Updated Feb 1, 2018
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Uri Gneezy; Agne Kajackaite; Joel Sobel (2018). Replication data for: Lying Aversion and the Size of the Lie [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.3886/E113165V1
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Feb 1, 2018
    Dataset provided by
    American Economic Association
    Authors
    Uri Gneezy; Agne Kajackaite; Joel Sobel
    Description

    This paper studies lying. An agent randomly picks a number from a known distribution. She can then report any number and receive a monetary payoff based only on her report. The paper presents a model of lying costs that generates hypotheses regarding behavior. In an experiment, we find that the highest fraction of lies is from reporting the maximal outcome, but some participants do not make the maximal lie. More participants lie partially when the experimenter cannot observe their outcomes than when the experimenter can verify the observed outcome. Partial lying increases when the prior probability of the highest outcome decreases.

  4. UK adults on lying on online dating sites and apps 2023

    • statista.com
    Updated May 22, 2024
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Statista (2024). UK adults on lying on online dating sites and apps 2023 [Dataset]. https://www.statista.com/statistics/1468543/uk-adults-lying-dating-sites-apps/
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    May 22, 2024
    Dataset authored and provided by
    Statistahttp://statista.com/
    Time period covered
    2023
    Area covered
    United Kingdom
    Description

    According to a survey conducted in the United Kingdom (UK) in 2023, 16 percent of online dating service users had lied about their age, while one in ten had lied about their name. Overall, eight percent had lied about their job, and four percent had lied about their current relationship status.

  5. U.S. survey on situations that warrant a lie 2016

    • statista.com
    Updated Apr 4, 2016
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Statista (2016). U.S. survey on situations that warrant a lie 2016 [Dataset]. https://www.statista.com/statistics/539795/situations-that-warrant-a-lie/
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Apr 4, 2016
    Dataset authored and provided by
    Statistahttp://statista.com/
    Time period covered
    Feb 9, 2016 - Feb 10, 2016
    Area covered
    United States
    Description

    This statistic shows the results of a survey among adult Americans in 2016 on situations in which lying is okay. During the survey, 18 percent of respondents stated that lying in order to avoid hurting someone's feelings is often okay, while 58 percent said it was sometimes okay, and 24 percent thought it was never okay to lie in order to avoid hurting someone's feelings.

  6. h

    Who never tells a lie? [Data set and Programs]

    • heidata.uni-heidelberg.de
    bin, pdf +1
    Updated Apr 5, 2017
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Christoph Vanberg; Christoph Vanberg (2017). Who never tells a lie? [Data set and Programs] [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.11588/DATA/10087
    Explore at:
    text/plain; charset=us-ascii(14678), text/plain; charset=us-ascii(17535), text/plain; charset=us-ascii(17534), text/plain; charset=us-ascii(17532), text/plain; charset=us-ascii(7842), pdf(349683), bin(30659), bin(34766), text/plain; charset=us-ascii(56927), text/plain; charset=us-ascii(66825)Available download formats
    Dataset updated
    Apr 5, 2017
    Dataset provided by
    heiDATA
    Authors
    Christoph Vanberg; Christoph Vanberg
    License

    https://heidata.uni-heidelberg.de/api/datasets/:persistentId/versions/1.1/customlicense?persistentId=doi:10.11588/DATA/10087https://heidata.uni-heidelberg.de/api/datasets/:persistentId/versions/1.1/customlicense?persistentId=doi:10.11588/DATA/10087

    Area covered
    Germany
    Description

    I experimentally investigate the hypothesis that many people avoid lying even in a situation where doing so would result in a Pareto improvement. Replicating (Erat and Gneezy, Management Science 58, 723-733, 2012), I find that a significant fraction of subjects tell the truth in a sender-receiver game where both subjects earn a higher payoff when the partner makes an incorrect guess regarding the roll of a die. However, a non-incentivized questionnaire indicates that the vast majority of these subjects expected their partner not to follow their message. I conduct two new experiments explicitly designed to test for a 'pure' aversion to lying, and find no evidence for the existence of such a motivation. I discuss the implications of the findings for moral behavior and rule following more generally.

  7. H

    Replication Data for: Why Do We Lie? Distinguishing Between Competing Lying...

    • dataverse.harvard.edu
    Updated Sep 13, 2021
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Paul Clist (2021). Replication Data for: Why Do We Lie? Distinguishing Between Competing Lying Theories [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/OXOGY3
    Explore at:
    CroissantCroissant is a format for machine-learning datasets. Learn more about this at mlcommons.org/croissant.
    Dataset updated
    Sep 13, 2021
    Dataset provided by
    Harvard Dataverse
    Authors
    Paul Clist
    License

    CC0 1.0 Universal Public Domain Dedicationhttps://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/
    License information was derived automatically

    Description

    This code and dataset replicate the results in Paul Clist and Ying-yi Hong (2019) Why Do We Lie? Distinguishing Between Competing Lying Theories CBESS working paper 19-03, available at: https://ueaeco.github.io/working-papers/papers/cbess/UEA-CBESS-19-03.pdf The second experiment was preregistered: https://www.socialscienceregistry.org/trials/3547

  8. n

    Data from: A novel algorithm to enhance P300 in single trials: application...

    • data.niaid.nih.gov
    • datadryad.org
    • +1more
    zip
    Updated Sep 12, 2015
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Junfeng Gao; Hongjun Tian; Yong Yang; Xiaoling Yu; Chenhong Li; Nini Rao (2015). A novel algorithm to enhance P300 in single trials: application to lie detection using F-score and SVM [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2qc64
    Explore at:
    zipAvailable download formats
    Dataset updated
    Sep 12, 2015
    Dataset provided by
    South Central Minzu University
    Jiangxi University of Finance and Economics
    Department of Information Engineering, Officers College of CAPF, People's Republic of China
    University of Electronic Science and Technology of China
    Nanjing Fullshare Superconducting Technology Co., Ltd., Nanjing, People's Republic of China
    Authors
    Junfeng Gao; Hongjun Tian; Yong Yang; Xiaoling Yu; Chenhong Li; Nini Rao
    License

    https://spdx.org/licenses/CC0-1.0.htmlhttps://spdx.org/licenses/CC0-1.0.html

    Area covered
    China
    Description

    The investigation of lie detection methods based on P300 potentials has drawn much interest in recent years. We presented a novel algorithm to enhance signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of P300 and applied it in lie detection to increase the classification accuracy. Thirty-four subjects were divided randomly into guilty and innocent groups, and the EEG signals on 14 electrodes were recorded. A novel spatial denoising algorithm (SDA) was proposed to reconstruct the P300 with a high SNR based on independent component analysis. The differences between the proposed method and our/other early published methods mainly lie in the extraction and feature selection method of P300. Three groups of features were extracted from the denoised waves; then, the optimal features were selected by the F-score method. Selected feature samples were finally fed into three classical classifiers to make a performance comparison. The optimal parameter values in the SDA and the classifiers were tuned using a grid-searching training procedure with cross-validation. The support vector machine (SVM) approach was adopted to combine with an F-score because this approach had the best performance. The presented model F-score_SVM reaches a significantly higher classification accuracy for P300 (specificity of 96.05%) and non-P300 (sensitivity of 96.11%) compared with the results obtained without using SDA and compared with the results obtained by other classification models. Moreover, a higher individual diagnosis rate can be obtained compared with previous methods, and the presented method requires only a small number of stimuli in the real testing application.

  9. S

    Southeast University Multimodal Lie Detection Dataset

    • scidb.cn
    Updated Mar 24, 2025
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Xu Xiaolin; Zheng Wenming; Lian Hailun; Li Sunan; Liu Jiateng; Liu Anbang; Lu Cheng; Zong Yuan; Liang Zongbao (2025). Southeast University Multimodal Lie Detection Dataset [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.57760/sciencedb.22548
    Explore at:
    CroissantCroissant is a format for machine-learning datasets. Learn more about this at mlcommons.org/croissant.
    Dataset updated
    Mar 24, 2025
    Dataset provided by
    Science Data Bank
    Authors
    Xu Xiaolin; Zheng Wenming; Lian Hailun; Li Sunan; Liu Jiateng; Liu Anbang; Lu Cheng; Zong Yuan; Liang Zongbao
    Description

    To address the lack of a Chinese context based lie detection dataset in current research, we have developed SEUMLD, which is the first publicly available multimodal lie detection dataset based on Chinese conversations. SEUMLD contains data in three modalities: video, audio, and electrocardiogram signals. In order to effectively stimulate the participants' motivation to lie, we designed a paradigm of simulated crime and simulated interrogation experiments. By recording multimodal signals of participants during simulated interrogation, SEUMLD collected data from 76 participants who had lived in a Chinese language environment for a long time, totaling 3224 conversations. This dataset provides coarse-grained annotation for identifying whether participants lie throughout the entire conversation, as well as fine-grained annotation for precise segmentation of each conversation.

  10. Data from: CBS News Lying Poll, May 1997

    • icpsr.umich.edu
    ascii, delimited, sas +2
    Updated Jul 16, 2008
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    CBS News (2008). CBS News Lying Poll, May 1997 [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR04494.v1
    Explore at:
    sas, spss, stata, delimited, asciiAvailable download formats
    Dataset updated
    Jul 16, 2008
    Dataset provided by
    Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Researchhttps://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/pages/
    Authors
    CBS News
    License

    https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/ICPSR/studies/4494/termshttps://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/ICPSR/studies/4494/terms

    Time period covered
    May 1997
    Area covered
    United States
    Description

    This special topic poll, fielded May 6-8, 1997, is part of a continuing series of monthly surveys that solicit public opinion on the presidency and on a range of other political and social issues. Respondents were asked to give their opinions of President Bill Clinton and his handling of the presidency. Views were sought on the events surrounding the 1996 Democratic fundraising activities and the White House's involvement in them, whether President Clinton and Vice President Gore did anything wrong or illegal, and whether Congress should investigate the matter. Respondents gave their opinions of Vice President Al Gore, Secretary of State Madeleine Albright, Speaker of the House Newt Gingrich, and how well members of the United States Congress were handling their jobs. Several questions asked how satisfied respondents were with their job, whether it was their dream job, and if not, what their dream job would be. Other questions addressed whether lying and keeping secrets was ever justified, how often respondents lied to others and were lied to, and their ability to tell a lie and detect when others were lying. Additional topics addressed the most important quality in a doctor, how concerned respondents were about germs, whether tobacco companies were telling the truth about the health risks of smoking, and whether they should be held legally responsible for smoking-related illness and deaths. Information was also collected on whether respondents smoked, whether they had a child in the ninth grade, and whether they identified themselves as multiracial. Demographic variables include sex, race, age, household income, education level, employment status, occupation, religious preference, frequency of religious attendance, political party affiliation, political philosophy, voter participation history and registration status, length of time living at current residence, the presence of children and teenagers in the household, and type of residential area (e.g., urban or rural).

  11. f

    Descriptive Statistics for Lying Subscales.

    • plos.figshare.com
    xls
    Updated Jun 9, 2023
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Heather Mann; Ximena Garcia-Rada; Daniel Houser; Dan Ariely (2023). Descriptive Statistics for Lying Subscales. [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109591.t001
    Explore at:
    xlsAvailable download formats
    Dataset updated
    Jun 9, 2023
    Dataset provided by
    PLOS ONE
    Authors
    Heather Mann; Ximena Garcia-Rada; Daniel Houser; Dan Ariely
    License

    Attribution 4.0 (CC BY 4.0)https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
    License information was derived automatically

    Description

    Note. Internal consistency (α), mean (), and standard deviation (s) statistics are presented for each of the four lying subscales, for P1 and P2 participant samples.Descriptive Statistics for Lying Subscales.

  12. Data for www.preferencesfortruthtelling.com

    • figshare.com
    • search.datacite.org
    png
    Updated Aug 22, 2019
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Johannes Abeler (2019). Data for www.preferencesfortruthtelling.com [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4981589.v9
    Explore at:
    pngAvailable download formats
    Dataset updated
    Aug 22, 2019
    Dataset provided by
    Figsharehttp://figshare.com/
    Authors
    Johannes Abeler
    License

    Attribution 4.0 (CC BY 4.0)https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
    License information was derived automatically

    Description

    Data for interactive graphs on www.preferencesfortruthtelling.comThe data are based on Abeler, Nosenzo, Raymond "Preferences for truth-telling" (published in Econometrica 2019)

  13. Survey on lies and misuse of facts in politics and media in Europe 2018, by...

    • statista.com
    Updated Jan 24, 2025
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Statista (2025). Survey on lies and misuse of facts in politics and media in Europe 2018, by country [Dataset]. https://www.statista.com/statistics/913922/fact-misuse-in-politics-and-media-in-europe/
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Jan 24, 2025
    Dataset authored and provided by
    Statistahttp://statista.com/
    Time period covered
    Jun 22, 2018 - Jul 6, 2018
    Area covered
    Europe
    Description

    This statistic illustrates the results of a survey regarding the public opinion on the amount of lying and misuse of facts in politics and media compared to 30 years ago in selected countries in Europe in 2018. According to data published by IPSOS, 61 percent of Turkish respondents thought that the amount of lying and misuse of facts in politics and the media had increased compared to 30 years ago.

  14. Data from: Do privacy assurances work? A study of truthfulness in healthcare...

    • zenodo.org
    • data.niaid.nih.gov
    • +1more
    csv, txt
    Updated Apr 28, 2023
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Tamara M. Masters; Tamara M. Masters; Mark Keith; Mark Keith; Rachel Hess; Rachel Hess; Jeffrey Jenkins; Jeffrey Jenkins (2023). Do privacy assurances work? A study of truthfulness in healthcare history data collection [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.qrfj6q5k8
    Explore at:
    txt, csvAvailable download formats
    Dataset updated
    Apr 28, 2023
    Dataset provided by
    Zenodohttp://zenodo.org/
    Authors
    Tamara M. Masters; Tamara M. Masters; Mark Keith; Mark Keith; Rachel Hess; Rachel Hess; Jeffrey Jenkins; Jeffrey Jenkins
    License

    CC0 1.0 Universal Public Domain Dedicationhttps://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/
    License information was derived automatically

    Description

    Patients often provide untruthful information about their health to avoid embarrassment, evade treatment, or prevent financial loss. Privacy disclosures (e.g. HIPAA) intended to dissuade privacy concerns may actually increase patient lying. We used new mouse tracking-based technology to detect lies through mouse movement (distance and time to response) and patient answer adjustment in an online controlled study of 611 potential patients, randomly assigned to one of six treatments. Treatments differed in the notices patients received before health information was requested, including notices about privacy, benefits of truthful disclosure, and risks of inaccurate disclosure. Increased time or distance of device mouse movement and greater adjustment of answers indicate less truthfulness. Mouse tracking revealed a significant overall effect (p < 0.001) by treatment on the time to reach their final choice. The control took the least time indicating greater truthfulness and the privacy + risk group took the longest indicating the least truthfulness. Privacy, risk, and benefit disclosure statements led to greater lying. These differences were moderated by gender. Mouse tracking results largely confirmed the answer adjustment lie detection method with an overall treatment effect (p < .0001) and gender differences (p < .0001) on truthfulness. Privacy notices led to decreased patient honesty. Privacy notices should perhaps be administered well before personal health disclosure is requested to minimize patient untruthfulness. Mouse tracking and answer adjustment appear to be healthcare lie-detection methods to enhance optimal diagnosis and treatment.

  15. u

    Data from: Background data for: “I regret lying” VS. “I regret that I lied”:...

    • investigacion.usc.gal
    • dataverse.no
    Updated 2024
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Romasanta, Raquel P.; Romasanta, Raquel P. (2024). Background data for: “I regret lying” VS. “I regret that I lied”: Variation in the clausal complementation profile of REGRET in American and British English [Dataset]. https://investigacion.usc.gal/documentos/67a9c7ae19544708f8c6f868
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    2024
    Authors
    Romasanta, Raquel P.; Romasanta, Raquel P.
    Description

    This dataset contains tabular files recording occurrences of the verb REGRET complemented by a that- or (S) -ing-complement clause (CC) in the GloWbE corpus. Tokens were retrieved using the online interface (https://www.english-corpora.org/glowbe/) and manually annotated for several syntactic and semantic variables (variety, text type, finiteness, subject in the main clause (MC), voice of the CC, meaning of the verb in the CC, subject in the CC, animacy of the subject in the CC, words in the CC, coreferentiality, intervening material, negation in the CC, temporal relation). See ReadMe file for more details. Related publication: Romasanta, Raquel P. 2022. “I regret lying” VS. “I regret that I lied”: Variation in the clausal complementation profile of REGRET in American and British English. Miscelánea 65: 37-58

  16. o

    Replication data for: On lies and hard truths

    • openicpsr.org
    Updated Jun 17, 2021
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Sascha Behnk; Ernesto Reuben (2021). Replication data for: On lies and hard truths [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.3886/E143161V1
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Jun 17, 2021
    Authors
    Sascha Behnk; Ernesto Reuben
    License

    Attribution 4.0 (CC BY 4.0)https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
    License information was derived automatically

    Description

    We run an experimental study using sender-receiver games to evaluate how senders' willingness to lie to others compares to their willingness to tell hard truths, i.e., promote an outcome that the sender know is unfair to the receiver without explicitly lying. Unlike in previous work on lying when it has consequences, we do not find that antisocial behavior is less frequent when it involves lying than when it does not. In fact, we find the opposite result in the setting where there is social contact between senders and receivers, and receivers have enough information to judge whether they have been treated unfairly. In this setting, we find that senders prefer to hide behind a lie and implement the antisocial outcome by being dishonest rather than by telling the truth. These results are consistent with social image costs depending on the social proximity between senders and receivers, especially when receivers can judge the kindness of the senders' actions.

  17. Raw data: Are we modular lying cues detectors? The answer is “yes,...

    • figshare.com
    xlsx
    Updated Jan 20, 2016
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Mathieu Arminjon (2016). Raw data: Are we modular lying cues detectors? The answer is “yes, sometimes”. [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1512355.v1
    Explore at:
    xlsxAvailable download formats
    Dataset updated
    Jan 20, 2016
    Dataset provided by
    Figsharehttp://figshare.com/
    Authors
    Mathieu Arminjon
    License

    Attribution 4.0 (CC BY 4.0)https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
    License information was derived automatically

    Description

    LABEL EXPLANATIONS FOR STUDY 1 and 2 OF MANUSCRIPT ENTITLED "Are we modular lying cues detectors? The answer is “yes, sometimes” BY ARMINJON ET AL

  18. c

    Complicity without connection or communication, experimental data

    • datacatalogue.cessda.eu
    Updated May 27, 2025
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Barr, A (2025). Complicity without connection or communication, experimental data [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-853002
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    May 27, 2025
    Dataset provided by
    University of Nottingham
    Authors
    Barr, A
    Time period covered
    Dec 31, 2012 - Sep 30, 2017
    Area covered
    United Kingdom
    Variables measured
    Individual
    Measurement technique
    Subjects were randomly matched into pairs. In each pair there is a Player A and a Player B. The players cannot identify or communicate with each other. Each player is asked to roll a fair six sided die once in private and report the outcome to the experimenter by writing it on a slip of paper. Each player’s report determines the monetary payoff of the other player in the pair. The experiment was conducted at the CeDEx laboratory, University of Nottingham, in May 2015. In total, 294 students, recruited through ORSEE (Greiner 2004), participated in the CG, NAc variant, and NMD treatment. Of these, 63% were females.
    Description

    We use a novel laboratory experiment involving a die rolling task embedded within a coordination game to investigate whether complicity can emerge when decision-making is simultaneous, the potential accomplices are strangers and neither communication nor signaling is possible. Then, by comparing the behavior observed in this original game to that in a variant in which die-roll reporting players are paired with passive players instead of other die-roll reporters, while everything else is held constant, we isolate the effect of having a potential accomplice on the likelihood of an individual acting immorally. We find that complicity can emerge between strangers in the absence of opportunities to communicate or signal and that having a potential accomplice increases the likelihood of an individual acting immorally.

    This network project brings together economists, psychologists, computer and complexity scientists from three leading centres for behavioural social science at Nottingham, Warwick and UEA. This group will lead a research programme with two broad objectives: to develop and test cross-disciplinary models of human behaviour and behaviour change; to draw out their implications for the formulation and evaluation of public policy. Foundational research will focus on three inter-related themes: understanding individual behaviour and behaviour change; understanding social and interactive behaviour; rethinking the foundations of policy analysis. The project will explore implications of the basic science for policy via a series of applied projects connecting naturally with the three themes. These will include: the determinants of consumer credit behaviour; the formation of social values; strategies for evaluation of policies affecting health and safety. The research will integrate theoretical perspectives from multiple disciplines and utilise a wide range of complementary methodologies including: theoretical modeling of individuals, groups and complex systems; conceptual analysis; lab and field experiments; analysis of large data sets. The Network will promote high quality cross-disciplinary research and serve as a policy forum for understanding behaviour and behaviour change.

  19. d

    Replication Data for: Lying for Trump? Elite Cue-Taking and Expressive...

    • search.dataone.org
    • dataverse.harvard.edu
    Updated Nov 8, 2023
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Shino, Enrijeta; Daniel A. Smith; Laura Uribe (2023). Replication Data for: Lying for Trump? Elite Cue-Taking and Expressive Responding on Vote Method [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/R419IQ
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Nov 8, 2023
    Dataset provided by
    Harvard Dataverse
    Authors
    Shino, Enrijeta; Daniel A. Smith; Laura Uribe
    Description

    Data and replication code for ``Lying for Trump? Elite Cue-Taking and Expressive Responding on Vote Method".

  20. d

    Data from: Lying in a 3T MRI scanner induces neglect-like spatial attention...

    • search.dataone.org
    • data.niaid.nih.gov
    • +2more
    Updated May 20, 2025
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Axel Lindner; Daniel Wiesen; Hans-Otto Karnath (2025). Lying in a 3T MRI scanner induces neglect-like spatial attention bias [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.6t1g1jx05
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    May 20, 2025
    Dataset provided by
    Dryad Digital Repository
    Authors
    Axel Lindner; Daniel Wiesen; Hans-Otto Karnath
    Time period covered
    Jan 1, 2021
    Description

    Exposing subjects to the magnetic field of a 3T MRI scanner stimulates the vestibular organ and thereby induces - besides a VOR - horizontal biases in visual search and in subjective straight ahead, which are similar to those seen in stroke patients with spatial neglect.

Share
FacebookFacebook
TwitterTwitter
Email
Click to copy link
Link copied
Close
Cite
Statista (2024). U.S. online dating service users lying on their profiles 2024 [Dataset]. https://www.statista.com/statistics/1481187/us-online-dating-users-lying-on-profiles/
Organization logo

U.S. online dating service users lying on their profiles 2024

Explore at:
Dataset updated
Jul 22, 2024
Dataset authored and provided by
Statistahttp://statista.com/
Time period covered
Mar 27, 2024 - Apr 1, 2024
Area covered
United States
Description

According to a survey conducted in April 2024 in the United States, one in five online dating service users had lied about their age on their dating profile, while 14 percent had lied about their income. A further 14 percent had lied about their hobbies and interests, and 12 percent had lied about their height.

Search
Clear search
Close search
Google apps
Main menu