2 datasets found
  1. f

    Evidence of Experimental Bias in the Life Sciences: Why We Need Blind Data...

    • figshare.com
    tiff
    Updated May 31, 2023
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Luke Holman; Megan L. Head; Robert Lanfear; Michael D. Jennions (2023). Evidence of Experimental Bias in the Life Sciences: Why We Need Blind Data Recording [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002190
    Explore at:
    tiffAvailable download formats
    Dataset updated
    May 31, 2023
    Dataset provided by
    PLOS Biology
    Authors
    Luke Holman; Megan L. Head; Robert Lanfear; Michael D. Jennions
    License

    Attribution 4.0 (CC BY 4.0)https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
    License information was derived automatically

    Description

    Observer bias and other “experimenter effects” occur when researchers’ expectations influence study outcome. These biases are strongest when researchers expect a particular result, are measuring subjective variables, and have an incentive to produce data that confirm predictions. To minimize bias, it is good practice to work “blind,” meaning that experimenters are unaware of the identity or treatment group of their subjects while conducting research. Here, using text mining and a literature review, we find evidence that blind protocols are uncommon in the life sciences and that nonblind studies tend to report higher effect sizes and more significant p-values. We discuss methods to minimize bias and urge researchers, editors, and peer reviewers to keep blind protocols in mind.

  2. d

    Prolific observer bias in the life sciences: why we need blind data...

    • search.dataone.org
    • researchdata.edu.au
    • +3more
    Updated Apr 12, 2025
    Share
    FacebookFacebook
    TwitterTwitter
    Email
    Click to copy link
    Link copied
    Close
    Cite
    Luke Holman; Megan L. Head; Robert Lanfear; Michael D. Jennions (2025). Prolific observer bias in the life sciences: why we need blind data recording [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.hn40n
    Explore at:
    Dataset updated
    Apr 12, 2025
    Dataset provided by
    Dryad Digital Repository
    Authors
    Luke Holman; Megan L. Head; Robert Lanfear; Michael D. Jennions
    Time period covered
    Jan 1, 2015
    Description

    Observer bias and other “experimenter effects†occur when researchers’ expectations influence study outcome. These biases are strongest when researchers expect a particular result, are measuring subjective variables, and have an incentive to produce data that confirm predictions. To minimize bias, it is good practice to work “blind,†meaning that experimenters are unaware of the identity or treatment group of their subjects while conducting research. Here, using text mining and a literature review, we find evidence that blind protocols are uncommon in the life sciences and that nonblind studies tend to report higher effect sizes and more significant p-values. We discuss methods to minimize bias and urge researchers, editors, and peer reviewers to keep blind protocols in mind.

  3. Not seeing a result you expected?
    Learn how you can add new datasets to our index.

Share
FacebookFacebook
TwitterTwitter
Email
Click to copy link
Link copied
Close
Cite
Luke Holman; Megan L. Head; Robert Lanfear; Michael D. Jennions (2023). Evidence of Experimental Bias in the Life Sciences: Why We Need Blind Data Recording [Dataset]. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002190

Evidence of Experimental Bias in the Life Sciences: Why We Need Blind Data Recording

Explore at:
120 scholarly articles cite this dataset (View in Google Scholar)
tiffAvailable download formats
Dataset updated
May 31, 2023
Dataset provided by
PLOS Biology
Authors
Luke Holman; Megan L. Head; Robert Lanfear; Michael D. Jennions
License

Attribution 4.0 (CC BY 4.0)https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
License information was derived automatically

Description

Observer bias and other “experimenter effects” occur when researchers’ expectations influence study outcome. These biases are strongest when researchers expect a particular result, are measuring subjective variables, and have an incentive to produce data that confirm predictions. To minimize bias, it is good practice to work “blind,” meaning that experimenters are unaware of the identity or treatment group of their subjects while conducting research. Here, using text mining and a literature review, we find evidence that blind protocols are uncommon in the life sciences and that nonblind studies tend to report higher effect sizes and more significant p-values. We discuss methods to minimize bias and urge researchers, editors, and peer reviewers to keep blind protocols in mind.

Search
Clear search
Close search
Google apps
Main menu